User login
VANCOUVER—Language proficiency affects a person’s results on the King-Devick concussion screening test, according to research presented at the 68th Annual Meeting of the American Academy of Neurology. In a study at New York University (NYU), it took 27 healthy, native English-speaking volunteers 42.8 seconds to complete the test, which is about average for nonconcussed subjects.
However, it took 27 other volunteers for whom English was a second language 54.4 seconds to complete the test. Had the test been given on the sidelines instead of in a laboratory, the extra 12 seconds might easily have been mistaken as a sign of serious concussion because concussions generally add about 5 seconds to the King-Devick score.
“A prolongation of 12 seconds in non-native English speakers has real clinical implications,” said lead investigator Katharine Dempsey, a medical student and member of the eye movement research team in the department of neurology at NYU.
Katharine Dempsey
The King-Devick test is an increasingly popular sideline screening tool used widely in professional sports. Subjects are timed as they read out loud and in English three sets of 40 numbers. Each set is progressively more difficult to read. The test is administered by flash cards or, as in the study, by computer.
In all, 18 languages were spoken by the group of non-native English speakers. The most common native languages in this group were Spanish and Chinese. All of the non-native speakers at NYU were proficient in English, but their native languages were often dominant, meaning that they were used at home and to perform mental arithmetic. Some subjects did not use Arabic numerals or read from right to left in their native tongues.
Instructions for the King-Devick tool recommend comparing subjects’ performance with their own preseason baseline scores; the NYU findings emphasize the importance of this technique when subjects aren’t native English speakers. The investigators point out that when baseline scores are unavailable, non-native English speakers may be scored against reference ranges for native speakers.“There’s incredible utility in using a sideline concussion screening test, but we definitely have to get out the message that the best practice is to take an athlete’s own preseason baseline. We have to be incredibly cautious when comparing test times of non-native English speakers to a normative database for native speakers,” Ms. Dempsey said.
The participants were in their early 30s, on average, and had no histories of concussion. The majority were women, and most were NYU employees or their friends.
The researchers also tracked eye movements during testing. Non-native speakers had more saccades (149 vs 135), but also fixated longer on numbers before initiating eye movement (345.4 milliseconds vs 288.0 milliseconds). Lag time correlated with native language dominance and suggests longer processing time.
The next step for research is to test how well patients perform on the King-Devick test in their native languages, Ms. Dempsey said.
—M. Alexander Otto
Suggested Reading
Galetta KM, Barrett J, Allen M, et al. The King-Devick test as a determinant of head trauma and concussion in boxers and MMA fighters. Neurology. 2011;76(17):1456-1462.
VANCOUVER—Language proficiency affects a person’s results on the King-Devick concussion screening test, according to research presented at the 68th Annual Meeting of the American Academy of Neurology. In a study at New York University (NYU), it took 27 healthy, native English-speaking volunteers 42.8 seconds to complete the test, which is about average for nonconcussed subjects.
However, it took 27 other volunteers for whom English was a second language 54.4 seconds to complete the test. Had the test been given on the sidelines instead of in a laboratory, the extra 12 seconds might easily have been mistaken as a sign of serious concussion because concussions generally add about 5 seconds to the King-Devick score.
“A prolongation of 12 seconds in non-native English speakers has real clinical implications,” said lead investigator Katharine Dempsey, a medical student and member of the eye movement research team in the department of neurology at NYU.
Katharine Dempsey
The King-Devick test is an increasingly popular sideline screening tool used widely in professional sports. Subjects are timed as they read out loud and in English three sets of 40 numbers. Each set is progressively more difficult to read. The test is administered by flash cards or, as in the study, by computer.
In all, 18 languages were spoken by the group of non-native English speakers. The most common native languages in this group were Spanish and Chinese. All of the non-native speakers at NYU were proficient in English, but their native languages were often dominant, meaning that they were used at home and to perform mental arithmetic. Some subjects did not use Arabic numerals or read from right to left in their native tongues.
Instructions for the King-Devick tool recommend comparing subjects’ performance with their own preseason baseline scores; the NYU findings emphasize the importance of this technique when subjects aren’t native English speakers. The investigators point out that when baseline scores are unavailable, non-native English speakers may be scored against reference ranges for native speakers.“There’s incredible utility in using a sideline concussion screening test, but we definitely have to get out the message that the best practice is to take an athlete’s own preseason baseline. We have to be incredibly cautious when comparing test times of non-native English speakers to a normative database for native speakers,” Ms. Dempsey said.
The participants were in their early 30s, on average, and had no histories of concussion. The majority were women, and most were NYU employees or their friends.
The researchers also tracked eye movements during testing. Non-native speakers had more saccades (149 vs 135), but also fixated longer on numbers before initiating eye movement (345.4 milliseconds vs 288.0 milliseconds). Lag time correlated with native language dominance and suggests longer processing time.
The next step for research is to test how well patients perform on the King-Devick test in their native languages, Ms. Dempsey said.
—M. Alexander Otto
VANCOUVER—Language proficiency affects a person’s results on the King-Devick concussion screening test, according to research presented at the 68th Annual Meeting of the American Academy of Neurology. In a study at New York University (NYU), it took 27 healthy, native English-speaking volunteers 42.8 seconds to complete the test, which is about average for nonconcussed subjects.
However, it took 27 other volunteers for whom English was a second language 54.4 seconds to complete the test. Had the test been given on the sidelines instead of in a laboratory, the extra 12 seconds might easily have been mistaken as a sign of serious concussion because concussions generally add about 5 seconds to the King-Devick score.
“A prolongation of 12 seconds in non-native English speakers has real clinical implications,” said lead investigator Katharine Dempsey, a medical student and member of the eye movement research team in the department of neurology at NYU.
Katharine Dempsey
The King-Devick test is an increasingly popular sideline screening tool used widely in professional sports. Subjects are timed as they read out loud and in English three sets of 40 numbers. Each set is progressively more difficult to read. The test is administered by flash cards or, as in the study, by computer.
In all, 18 languages were spoken by the group of non-native English speakers. The most common native languages in this group were Spanish and Chinese. All of the non-native speakers at NYU were proficient in English, but their native languages were often dominant, meaning that they were used at home and to perform mental arithmetic. Some subjects did not use Arabic numerals or read from right to left in their native tongues.
Instructions for the King-Devick tool recommend comparing subjects’ performance with their own preseason baseline scores; the NYU findings emphasize the importance of this technique when subjects aren’t native English speakers. The investigators point out that when baseline scores are unavailable, non-native English speakers may be scored against reference ranges for native speakers.“There’s incredible utility in using a sideline concussion screening test, but we definitely have to get out the message that the best practice is to take an athlete’s own preseason baseline. We have to be incredibly cautious when comparing test times of non-native English speakers to a normative database for native speakers,” Ms. Dempsey said.
The participants were in their early 30s, on average, and had no histories of concussion. The majority were women, and most were NYU employees or their friends.
The researchers also tracked eye movements during testing. Non-native speakers had more saccades (149 vs 135), but also fixated longer on numbers before initiating eye movement (345.4 milliseconds vs 288.0 milliseconds). Lag time correlated with native language dominance and suggests longer processing time.
The next step for research is to test how well patients perform on the King-Devick test in their native languages, Ms. Dempsey said.
—M. Alexander Otto
Suggested Reading
Galetta KM, Barrett J, Allen M, et al. The King-Devick test as a determinant of head trauma and concussion in boxers and MMA fighters. Neurology. 2011;76(17):1456-1462.
Suggested Reading
Galetta KM, Barrett J, Allen M, et al. The King-Devick test as a determinant of head trauma and concussion in boxers and MMA fighters. Neurology. 2011;76(17):1456-1462.